Humans more similar than different. Nature Download citation. Published : 20 December Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:. Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative. Advanced search. Skip to main content Thank you for visiting nature. Download PDF. Ethnicity may be as good a guide to genetic susceptibilities as testing. You have full access to this article via your institution. This might sometimes cause people to neglect to report common instantiations that are not salient.
That is, treating students or job applicants equally is something that would directly affect the British participants, whereas equal treatment of Black people does not bearing in mind that most of the British participants were Caucasian. Although these observations are speculative, they show that open-ended measures of concept mapping, as used here and in past research, are likely to be unreliable as sole measures of the typicality of an exemplar.
Another issue is that although some of the observed differences in instantiations are clearly explicable in terms of contextual factors, others are more difficult to explain.
Water conservation is an aspect of daily life in the region where this research was conducted Karnataka , making it highly relevant to the residents. However, they did not spontaneously think of this behavior in relation to environmental protection.
This may be a case where an instantiation is taken for granted, making it less salient to respondents Gricean maxims; Grice, Alternatively, it may be the case that water conservation is seen as a basic necessity rather than a way to protect the environment.
As a result, Indian participants may have perceived water shortage as a personal challenge rather than a challenge to the environment. A further limitation pertains to the samples used. Because most participants were students in specific regions of each nation, generalizing to the population of each country should be done with caution cf.
Hanel and Vione, For example, Brazilians mentioned passing entrance exams for prestigious jobs as an instantiation. However, it is less likely that people who are close to retirement would also regard this as an instantiation for success. Further, although the instantiations are in general not in line with typical gender stereotypes, similar limitations may pertain to the large proportion of female participants in all samples.
Finally, the answers in the Indian data were more heterogeneous i. Most of the Indian participants did not have English as a first language, although English was the language of instruction both in school and at university.
As a result, English proficiency varied substantially between participants. Another possible explanation for the difficulties we had in parsing the Indian responses is that Indian participants used a line of thought that was too unique for us to follow.
This is sometimes a problem in anthropological research Barley, We sought to minimize the extent of this problem by working closely with our Indian collaborator. Despite the difficulties in interpreting the Indian data, we did not exclude it because excluding conditions is perceived to be bad practice Simmons et al. We aimed to get an overview of typical instantiations across 23 values and 3 countries. However, because of large within- and surprisingly small between-country variabilities in combination with relatively small samples sizes of around 30 participants in each country, cross-cultural comparisons were difficult.
Thus, future research might want to measure instantiations in larger samples to detect potential small effects of group membership e. Larger sample sizes would also allow one to test for moderators.
For example, do left- and right-wingers instantiate conservation and openness values differently? A further possibility is to ask participants to describe three situations in the past in which they applied the value themselves or have seen applications of the value.
The aim of Study 2 was to test the extent to which the instantiations obtained in Study 1 would be recognized as being promoted by the specific value that had elicited them. Most instantiations were correctly matched in both the United Kingdom and Brazil, indicating a relatively similar understanding of which instantiations are related to which values.
Interestingly, participants were often able to correctly match instantiations that their compatriots had not mentioned in the free recall procedure used in Study 1. Thus, the findings of Studies 1 and 2 converge with evidence from cognitive psychology indicating that most people are able to recognize instances of a category, even when the instances are atypical; for example, people can label an ostrich or a penguin as members of the bird category, even though these birds are seldom the first examples that come to mind when participants were asked to name birds e.
Hence, the instantiations that have been correctly matched can be regarded as valid instantiations, but are potentially atypical when they were not spontaneously generated in Study 1. An obvious limitation of Study 2 is the use of fixed response alternatives, i.
Although five of the six values were chosen randomly with the remaining value being the one related to the instantiation , they were the same across participants and countries for all six instantiations of each value.
Consequently, although we can compare findings between participants and regions, we cannot do so between value instantiations and values. If the five alternative values had been selected out a broader range of values e. However, between-country comparisons may also be moderated by the choice of response alternatives. It might be the case that the nature of the differences between countries depends on which response options are offered.
Nonetheless, given that these options were chosen randomly, there is no reason to suspect any systematic effect of the options on the between-country comparisons. Our results suggest that some behaviors are more closely associated with some values than other behaviors. Thus, an unanswered question is whether the value-behavior link is moderated by the typicality of an instantiation behavior.
This issue is theoretically important because it points to different ways in which typicality might affect the role of values in behavior. The ART postulates, based on previous findings of the authors e. As argued above, both personal experiences and social-contextual factors influence the extent to which a behavior is a prominent instantiation of values. This, in turn, leads to the activation of one or more values that influence which behavior is chosen in a specific situation cf.
Thus, not only the attitude-behavior link should be moderated by typicality, but also the value-behavior link: If an instantiation here: behavior or behavioral intention is more closely linked to a value, the two are more strongly associated. It is important to know whether typicality matters, because it allows us to better predict when values are correlated with behavior.
For example, one might expect protecting the environment predicts saving water in the United Kingdom but not in Brazil. In conclusion, we hope that our findings allow researchers to develop more specific hypotheses in which context and for which sample type a value predicts a behavior. Overall, Study 1 revealed that most examples that are spontaneously attached to values vary in how much they are shaped by context. In most cases, within-country variability outweighed between-country differences.
Nevertheless, many of the instances for which between-country differences were found could be linked to contextual factors. In Study 2, we found that most instantiations that had been spontaneously produced by participants in another country could reliably be matched to the values that they exemplified.
In other words, people endorse the same values to a similar extent across countries and also instantiate them similarly. We hope this research helps to lay a foundation for future research examining these differences and their implications for intercultural understanding and communication. Both studies are described in the Ph. GM and AM: conceptualization. PH and AS: visualizations. This study was financial support by the School of Psychology, Cardiff University psych.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. Anderson, R. Instantiation of general terms. Ashton, M. A six-factor structure of personality-descriptive adjectives: solutions from psycholexical studies in seven languages.
Bandura, A. Transmission of aggression through imitation of aggressive models. Barley, N. London: Penguin Books. Google Scholar. Bilsky, W. Cross Cult.
Bobo, L. Carley, K. Asher Edinburgh: Pergamon Press , — Collins, A. Retrieval time from semantic memory. Fazio, R. Measuring associative strength: category-item associations and their activation from memory.
Fehr, B. Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype perspective. Fischer, R. Whence differences in value priorities? Individual, cultural, or artifactual sources. Goldberg, L. The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure. Gollwitzer, P. Admission of failure and symbolic self-completion: Extending Lewinian theory.
Gouveia, V. Teoria funcionalista dos valores humanos [Functional theory of human values: Fundaments, applications and perspectives]. Grice, H. Cole and J. Grouzet, F. The structure of goal contents across 15 cultures. Gurven, M. How universal is the Big Five? Testing the five-factor model of personality variation among forager—farmers in the Bolivian Amazon. An observed g r within the envelope indicates no association at scale r.
We determined the relationship between the total basal area of focal species and average species number or average individual number in the neighborhood of focal species Zhang et al. For each individual of all species in the plot, neighborhoods with radius of 5 m from the focal tree were defined, and the values for each factor were calculated. N is the sum of basal area of all species in the same quadrat. The spatial patterns of congeneric species were similar in both plots, which all showed significant aggregation under CSR Figure 3.
Spatial patterns of Quercus serrata var. The spatial patterns of Quercus species are contrasted with complete spatial randomness null model CSR using the g r function. The black solid line indicates the observed value, and the black dashed line indicates the theoretical value.
The spatial patterns of Quercus species are contrasted with heterogeneous Poisson's process null model HP using the g r function.
The similarities in spatial distributions of congeneric species were different between plots. However, in Plot 2 Figure 6 , the spatial distribution of one congeneric species was often different from that of other congeneric species.
Similarity in spatial distributions of Quercus serrata var. Figure 7 c,d, and f. Interspecific associations of Quercus serrata var. The g 12 r and heterogeneous Poisson's process null model are used to calculate interspecific associations of Quercus species. Interspecific associations of each congeneric species showed some similarities in Plot 1 Table 2. Both Quercus species showed negative associations with eight of the other 10 species and no association with two species.
However, they showed significant negative associations only with four species. Interspecific associations were asymmetrical. Eight of the other 10 species had no significant influences to the Quercus species.
Only Sorbus alnifolia showed significant negative association to both Quercus species. Pyrus calleryana showed significant negative association to QS. QS means Q. However, interspecific associations of each congeneric species were different in Plot 2 Table 3. Although all three Quercus species showed negative associations with seven of the other 10 species, only a few associations were significant: QS showed significant negative association with Bothrocaryum controversum and Sorbus folgneri , QV showed significant negative association with Forsythia suspensa and significant positive association with Spiraea dasyantha , and QA showed significant negative association with B.
At the same time, six of the other 10 species showed significant negative associations with QS, two species showed significant associations with QV, and no species showed significant associations with QA. The interspecific associations of the other 10 species to QS and to QV all showed no significant differences. In Plot 2, however, we found that the interspecific associations of each Quercus species were not exactly the same on each scale.
The interspecific associations of each Quercus species showed more differences on each scale in Plot 2 than in Plot 1 Table 4. Comparisons of interspecific associations of each Quercus species with one species in Plot 2. In both plots, the Quercus species were all abundant. They all had less neighborhood richness around them. However, only in Plot 1 was the number of neighborhood species significantly negatively correlated with the total basal area of focal species, and the number of neighborhood individuals had no correlation with the total basal area Figure 8 a and c.
In Plot 2, neither the number of neighborhood species nor the number of neighborhood individuals was correlated with the total basal area of the focal species Figure 8 b and d.
At the same time, Plot 1 had lower diversity than Plot 2: the Shannon index was 1. Neighborhood analysis of all species in Plot 1 a and c and in Plot 2 b and d. The closely related Quercus species all showed aggregation spatial patterns under CSR, and the aggregations were weaker under HP in both plots. This finding indicates that the distributions of Quercus species were more consistent with HP than with CSR, that is, the distributions were more or less affected by the environment.
At the same time, the spatial distribution of Quercus species in Plot 1 showed no significant difference Figure 5 , suggesting similar resource utilization. The similarities in the interspecific associations of congeneric species were discovered for the first time. This finding indicates that congeneric species not only have similar responses to the habitat but also are likely to interact with the same species in the community Zhang et al.
Strangely, similarities in congeneric species did not necessarily cause strong competition among them. However, the Quercus species were not likely to compete with each other as there were positive association in Plot 1 Figure 7 a and b but they may have competition as there were negative association in Plot 2 Figure 7 c—e.
This phenomenon may be related to other environmental factors, such as soil conditions. Burns and Strauss also found that close relatives can compete more with each other or might have more mutualistic relationships than distant relatives under different soil conditions.
As the spatial patterns of Quercus species are affected by the environment, these congeneric species use the same resources without competition when the resources in the soil are abundant Plot 1. This finding is probably because Quercus species did not compete with each other and influenced the same species on the same scales in Plot 1 Figure 7 a and b.
Given the weak competition of Quercus species, they were not able to exclude noncongeneric species, so species diversity was high in Plot 2. Quercus species were the most abundant species in terms of basal area, but they had less diverse local communities in Plot 1.
However, it was not an artifact of Quercus species to have fewer individuals in the neighborhood Figure 8. Given that basal area was not correlated with the number of individuals, more conspecific individuals may be found around Quercus species than heterospecific individuals. Obviously, Quercus species have different survival strategies under different habitats. Congeneric species have some similar ecological characteristics such as similar spatial pattern and spatial distribution.
We also discovered similarities in the interspecific associations of congeneric species for the first time. Although the similarity in the use of resources may lead to competition, similar influence to noncongeneric species can provide an opportunity for congeneric species to strengthen their competitive ability and promote their coexistence. Thus, to obtain sufficient resources, congenerics may compete with each other until they have enough differentiation in resource utilization, or they may combine together to exclude the noncongeneric species to obtain more living space.
However, under different environments, congeneric species may change their survival strategies. Environmental factors and similar interspecific associations can affect the coexistence of congeneric species. However, which factor plays the most important role and in which way do these factors affect the coexistence still need further research. All authors contributed critically to the drafts and gave final approval for publication. They should not be confused with the evolutionary meanings. A homoplasious similarity is one that can be explained by a shared way of life.
The wings of birds and bats have developed separately: this can be explained by their similar functional requirements.
0コメント